Cognitive Abilities in Preterm and Term-Born Adolescents

      Objective

      To investigate the influence of a range of prenatal and postnatal factors on cognitive development in preterm and term-born adolescents.

      Study design

      Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities were used to assess general intellectual ability and 6 broad cognitive abilities in 145 young adolescents aged approximately 12.5 years and born 25-41 weeks gestational age (GA). To study potential links between neurophysiologic and cognitive outcomes, corticomotor excitability was measured using transcranial magnetic stimulation and surface electromyography. The influence of various prenatal and postnatal factors on cognitive development was investigated using relative importance regression modeling.

      Results

      Adolescents with greater GA tended to have better cognitive abilities (particularly general intellectual ability, working memory, and cognitive efficiency) and higher corticomotor excitability. Corticomotor excitability explained a higher proportion of the variance in cognitive outcome than GA. But the strongest predictors of cognitive outcome were combinations of prenatal and postnatal factors, particularly degree of social disadvantage at the time of birth, birthweight percentile, and height at assessment.

      Conclusions

      In otherwise neurologically healthy adolescents, GA accounts for little interindividual variability in cognitive abilities. The association between corticomotor excitability and cognitive performance suggests that reduced connectivity, potentially associated with brain microstructural abnormalities, may contribute to cognitive deficits in preterm children. It remains to be determined if the effects of low GA on cognitive outcomes attenuate over childhood in favor of a concomitant increase in the relative importance of heritability, or alternatively, if cognitive development is more heavily influenced by the quality of the postnatal environment.
      BW% ( Birthweight percentile), GA ( Gestational age), GIA ( General intellectual ability), IRSD ( Index of relative socioeconomic disadvantage), M1 ( Motor cortex), MRI ( Magnetic resonance imaging), rMT ( Resting motor threshold), SES ( Socioeconomic status), TMS ( Transcranial magnetic stimulation), WCH ( Women's and Children's Hospital)
      To read this article in full you will need to make a payment

      References

        • Beck S.
        • Wojdyla D.
        • Say L.
        • Betran A.P.
        • Merialdi M.
        • Requejo J.H.
        • et al.
        The worldwide incidence of preterm birth: a systematic review of maternal mortality and morbidity.
        Bull World Health Organ. 2010; 88: 31-38
        • Chyi L.J.
        • Lee H.C.
        • Hintz S.R.
        • Gould J.B.
        • Sutcliffe T.L.
        School outcomes of late preterm infants: special needs and challenges for infants born at 32 to 36 weeks gestation.
        J Pediatr. 2008; 153: 25-31
        • Morse S.B.
        • Zheng H.
        • Tang Y.
        • Roth J.
        Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants.
        Pediatrics. 2009; 123: e622-e629
        • Pitcher J.B.
        • Riley A.M.
        • Doeltgen S.H.
        • Kurylowicz L.
        • Rothwell J.C.
        • McAllister S.M.
        • et al.
        Physiological evidence consistent with reduced neuroplasticity in human adolescents born preterm.
        J Neurosci. 2012; 32: 16410-16416
        • Woythaler M.A.
        • McCormick M.C.
        • Smith V.C.
        Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants.
        Pediatrics. 2011; 127: e622-e629
        • Kirkegaard I.
        • Obel C.
        • Hedegaard M.
        • Henriksen T.B.
        Gestational age and birth weight in relation to school performance of 10-year-old children: a follow-up study of children born after 32 completed Weeks.
        Pediatrics. 2006; 118: 1600-1606
        • Lindstrom K.
        • Winbladh B.
        • Haglund B.
        • Hjern A.
        Preterm infants as young adults: a Swedish national cohort study.
        Pediatrics. 2007; 120: 70-77
        • van Baar A.L.
        • Vermaas J.
        • Knots E.
        • de Kleine M.J.
        • Soons P.
        Functioning at school age of moderately preterm children born at 32 to 36 weeks' gestational age.
        Pediatrics. 2009; 124: 251-257
        • Anderson P.
        • Doyle L.W.
        Neurobehavioral outcomes of school-age children born extremely low birth weight or very preterm in the 1990s.
        JAMA. 2003; 289: 3264-3272
        • Constable R.T.
        • Ment L.R.
        • Vohr B.R.
        • Kesler S.R.
        • Fulbright R.K.
        • Lacadie C.
        • et al.
        Prematurely born children demonstrate white matter microstructural differences at 12 years of age, relative to term control subjects: an investigation of group and gender effects.
        Pediatrics. 2008; 121: 306-316
        • Kesler S.R.
        • Reiss A.L.
        • Vohr B.
        • Watson C.
        • Schneider K.C.
        • Katz K.H.
        • et al.
        Brain volume reductions within multiple cognitive systems in male preterm children at age twelve.
        J Pediatr. 2008; 152: 513-520.e1
        • Counsell S.J.
        • Edwards A.D.
        • Chew A.T.
        • Anjari M.
        • Dyet L.E.
        • Srinivasan L.
        • et al.
        Specific relations between neurodevelopmental abilities and white matter microstructure in children born preterm.
        Brain. 2008; 131: 3201-3208
        • Ludeman N.A.
        • Berman J.I.
        • Wu Y.W.
        • Jeremy R.J.
        • Kornak J.
        • Bartha A.I.
        • et al.
        Diffusion tensor imaging of the pyramidal tracts in infants with motor dysfunction.
        Neurology. 2008; 71: 1676-1682
        • Pitcher J.B.
        • Schneider L.A.
        • Burns N.R.
        • Drysdale J.L.
        • Higgins R.D.
        • Ridding M.C.
        • et al.
        Reduced corticomotor excitability and motor skills development in children born preterm.
        J Physiol. 2012; 590: 5827-5844
        • D'Ausilio A.
        • Pulvermüller F.
        • Salmas P.
        • Bufalari I.
        • Begliomini C.
        • Fadiga L.
        The motor somatotopy of speech perception.
        Curr Biol. 2009; 19: 381-385
        • Floel A.
        • Cohen L.G.
        Contribution of noninvasive cortical stimulation to the study of memory functions.
        Brain Res Rev. 2007; 53: 250-259
        • Kloppel S.
        • Baumer T.
        • Kroeger J.
        • Koch M.A.
        • Buchel C.
        • Munchau A.
        • et al.
        The cortical motor threshold reflects microstructural properties of cerebral white matter.
        Neuroimage. 2008; 40: 1782-1791
        • Corsi D.J.
        • Davey Smith G.
        • Subramanian S.V.
        Commentary: challenges to establishing the link between birthweight and cognitive development.
        Int J Epidemiol. 2013; 42: 172-175
        • Hackman D.A.
        • Farah M.J.
        • Meaney M.J.
        Socioeconomic status and the brain: mechanistic insights from human and animal research.
        Nat Rev Neurosci. 2010; 11: 651-659
        • Pitcher J.
        • Schneider L.
        • Higgins R.
        • Burns N.
        • Nettelbeck T.
        • Ridding M.
        • et al.
        Motor cortex development and specific cognitive outcomes in children born 25-41 weeks gestation; preliminary findings of the PREMOCODE study.
        Acta Paediatr. 2009; 98: 210
        • Wassermann E.M.
        Risk and safety of repetitive transcranial magnetic stimulation: report and suggested guidelines from the International Workshop on the Safety of Repetitive Transcranial Magnetic Stimulation, June 5-7, 1996.
        Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1998; 108: 1-16
      1. Gardosi JO, Francis A. Customized Centile Calculator – GROW-Centile. 5.1 ed: Gestation Network, Available at: www.gestation.net. Accessed April 8 2008.

        • Woodcock R.
        • McGrew K.
        • Mather M.
        Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities.
        Riverside Publishing, Itasca, IL2001
        • Mather N.
        • Woodcock R.W.
        Examiner's Manual.
        Woodcock-Johnson III Tests of Cognitive Abilities. Riverside Publishing, Itasca, IL2001
        • Carroll J.
        Human cognitive abilities: a survey of factor analytic studies.
        Cambridge University Press, Cambridge1993
        • Rossini P.M.
        • Barker A.T.
        • Berardelli A.
        • Caramia M.D.
        • Caruso G.
        • Cracco R.Q.
        • et al.
        Non-invasive electrical and magnetic stimulation of the brain, spinal cord and roots—basic principles and procedures for routine clinical application - report of an IFCN committee.
        Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol. 1994; 91: 79-92
        • Oldfield R.C.
        The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh inventory.
        Neuropsychologia. 1971; 9: 97-113
        • Grömping U.
        Estimators of relative importance in linear regression based on variance decomposition.
        Am Stat. 2007; 61: 139-147
        • Lindeman R.H.
        • Merenda P.F.
        • Gold R.Z.
        Introduction to bivariate and multivariate analysis.
        Scott, Foresman, Glenview, IL1980
        • Donoghue D.L.D.
        • Morgan G.
        • Beard J.
        Influences on the degree of preterm birth in New South Wales.
        Aust N Z J Public Health. 2013; 37: 562-567
        • Lepage J.F.
        • Clouchoux C.
        • Lassonde M.
        • Evans A.C.
        • Deal C.L.
        • Theoret H.
        Abnormal motor cortex excitability is associated with reduced cortical thickness in X monosomy.
        Hum Brain Mapp. 2013; 34: 936-944
        • Granert O.
        • Peller M.
        • Gaser C.
        • Groppa S.
        • Hallett M.
        • Knutzen A.
        • et al.
        Manual activity shapes structure and function in contralateral human motor hand area.
        Neuroimage. 2011; 54: 32-41
        • Bracewell M.
        • Marlow N.
        Patterns of motor disability in very preterm children.
        Ment Retard Dev Disabil Res Rev. 2002; 8: 241-248
        • Bengtsson S.L.
        • Nagy Z.
        • Skare S.
        • Forsman L.
        • Forssberg H.
        • Ullen F.
        Extensive piano practicing has regionally specific effects on white matter development.
        Nat Neurosci. 2005; 8: 1148-1150
        • Brant A.M.
        • Munakata Y.
        • Boomsma D.I.
        • Defries J.C.
        • Haworth C.M.
        • Keller M.C.
        • et al.
        The nature and nurture of high IQ: an extended sensitive period for intellectual development.
        Psychol Sci. 2013; 24: 1487-1495
        • Case A.
        • Paxson C.
        Stature and status: height, ability, and labor market outcomes.
        J Polit Econ. 2008; 116: 499-532
        • Hanson J.L.
        • Hair N.
        • Shen D.G.
        • Shi F.
        • Gilmore J.H.
        • Wolfe B.L.
        • et al.
        Family poverty affects the rate of human infant brain growth.
        PLoS One. 2013; 8: e80954
        • Jurado M.B.
        • Rosselli M.
        The elusive nature of executive functions: a review of our current understanding.
        Neuropsychol Rev. 2007; 17: 213-233
        • Malhotra P.
        • Coulthard E.J.
        • Husain M.
        Role of right posterior parietal cortex in maintaining attention to spatial locations over time.
        Brain. 2009; 132: 645-660
        • Jednoróg K.
        • Altarelli I.
        • Monzalvo K.
        • Fluss J.
        • Dubois J.
        • Billard C.
        • et al.
        The influence of socioeconomic status on children's brain structure.
        PLoS One. 2012; 7: e42486
        • Arcangeli T.
        • Thilaganathan B.
        • Hooper R.
        • Khan K.S.
        • Bhide A.
        Neurodevelopmental delay in small babies at term: a systematic review.
        Ultrasound Obstet Gynecol. 2012; 40: 267-275
        • Leitner Y.
        • Heldman D.
        • Harel S.
        • Pick C.G.
        Deficits in spatial orientation of children with intrauterine growth retardation.
        Brain Res Bull. 2005; 67: 13-18
        • Cheong J.L.Y.
        • Hunt R.W.
        • Anderson P.J.
        • Howard K.
        • Thompson D.K.
        • Wang H.X.
        • et al.
        Head growth in preterm infants: correlation with magnetic resonance imaging and neurodevelopmental outcome.
        Pediatrics. 2008; 121: e1534-e1540
        • Hack M.
        • Breslau N.
        • Weissman B.
        • Aram D.
        • Klein N.
        • Borawski E.
        Effect of very low birth weight and subnormal head size on cognitive abilities at school age.
        N Engl J Med. 1991; 325: 231-237
        • Neubauer V.
        • Griesmaier E.
        • Pehbock-Walser N.
        • Pupp-Peglow U.
        • Kiechl-Kohlendorfer U.
        Poor postnatal head growth in very preterm infants is associated with impaired neurodevelopment outcome.
        Acta Paediatr. 2013; 102: 883-888
        • Belfort M.B.
        • Rifas-Shiman S.L.
        • Sullivan T.
        • Collins C.T.
        • McPhee A.J.
        • Ryan P.
        • et al.
        Infant growth before and after term: effects on neurodevelopment in preterm infants.
        Pediatrics. 2011; 128: e899-e906
        • Kuban K.C.
        • Allred E.N.
        • O'Shea T.M.
        • Paneth N.
        • Westra S.
        • Miller C.
        • et al.
        Developmental correlates of head circumference at birth and two years in a cohort of extremely low gestational age newborns.
        J Pediatr. 2009; 155: 344-349
        • Brumback T.Y.
        • Arbel Y.
        • Donchin E.
        • Goldman M.S.
        Efficiency of responding to unexpected information varies with sex, age, and pubertal development in early adolescence.
        Psychophysiology. 2012; 49: 1330-1339
        • Lambourne K.
        The relationship between working memory capacity and physical activity rates in young adults.
        J Sports Sci Med. 2006; 5: 149-153
        • Longman R.S.
        • Saklofske D.H.
        • Fung T.S.
        WAIS-III percentile scores by education and sex for US and Canadian populations.
        Assessment. 2007; 14: 426-432
        • Kaufman S.B.
        Sex differences in mental rotation and spatial visualization ability: can they be accounted for by differences in working memory capacity?.
        Intelligence. 2007; 35: 211-223
        • Mathiasen R.
        • Hansen B.M.
        • Andersen A.M.
        • Forman J.L.
        • Greisen G.
        Gestational age and basic school achievements: a national follow-up study in Denmark.
        Pediatrics. 2010; 126: e1553-e1561
        • Marlow N.
        • Wolke D.
        • Bracewell M.A.
        • Samara M.
        • the EPICure Study Group
        Neurologic and developmental disability at six years of age after extremely preterm birth.
        N Engl J Med. 2005; 352: 9-19
        • Smith M.J.
        • Keel J.C.
        • Greenberg B.D.
        • Adams L.F.
        • Schmidt P.J.
        • Rubinow D.A.
        • et al.
        Menstrual cycle effects on cortical excitability.
        Neurology. 1999; 53: 2069-2072